Expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension: A case of english for specific purposes (esp)
Cognitive Load Theory assists researchers in designing instructional procedures that can lead to enhancement of reading skills. This paper aims to examine cognitive load effect as expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension of English for Specific Purposes (ESP). An experiment was designed to investigate whether the expertise reversal effect can be applied to reading comprehension of ESP. The implications of the experiment findings can be used in teaching and learning ESP reading comprehension. The findings will help instructors design more appropriate reading comprehension instructions with alternative versions to integrate different domains such as English for Geography and Mathematics effectively and to test the expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension
Trang 1
Trang 2
Trang 3
Trang 4
Trang 5
Trang 6
Trang 7
Trang 8
Trang 9
Trang 10
Tóm tắt nội dung tài liệu: Expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension: A case of english for specific purposes (esp)
74 Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 EXPERTISE REVERSAL EFFECT ON READING COMPREHENSION: A CASE OF ENGLISH FOR SPECIFIC PURPOSES (ESP) HUYNH CONG MINH HUNG Ho Chi Minh City Open University, Vietnam – hung.hcm@ou.edu.vn (Received: June 30, 2017; Revised: November 07, 2017; Accepted: November 29, 2017) ABSTRACT Cognitive Load Theory assists researchers in designing instructional procedures that can lead to enhancement of reading skills. This paper aims to examine cognitive load effect as expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension of English for Specific Purposes (ESP). An experiment was designed to investigate whether the expertise reversal effect can be applied to reading comprehension of ESP. The implications of the experiment findings can be used in teaching and learning ESP reading comprehension. The findings will help instructors design more appropriate reading comprehension instructions with alternative versions to integrate different domains such as English for Geography and Mathematics effectively and to test the expertise reversal effect on reading comprehension. Keywords: Cognitive Load Theory; Expertise reversal effect. 1. Introduction Cognitive Load Theory (CLT) has developed since the 1980s and attracted many researchers all over the world. CLT is concerned with the limitation of working memory. According to CLT, reading comprehension is defined as a constraint of a limited working memory (Eskey and Grabe, 1988). It will be more difficult for learners if working memory goes beyond its limitations (Goldman, Varma and Cote, 1996). Another difficulty for reading comprehension is the various levels of readers. According to Daneman and Capenter (1983) and Perfetti (1985), low level readers who do not have enough automation of schemas in reading comprehension may generate increased cognitive load. Obviously, differences between high level readers (experts) and low level readers (novices) are explained by using levels of expertise (Chi, Feltovich and Glasser, 1981). There are several instructional effects generated by CLT as the expertise reversal effect when instructions useful for novices may be unhelpful for more expert readers (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler and Sweller, 2007). The Expertise Reversal Effect is examined not only in natural sciences but also in well-structured domains like literacy texts (Kalyuga and Renkl, 2010) and biology texts (McNamara, Kintsch, Songer, 1996). The results of McNamara et al.’s (1996) experiments showed that novices would benefit from information added to original instructional text while experts were beneficial from original instructional text (McNamara et al., ibid). Oksa, et al. (2010) used Shakespearean text to differentiate instructional effectiveness and found that it was difficult for novices to comprehend the text, which used a lot of sophisticated Elizabethan English language. McNamara et al. (ibid) investigated the effect of text cohesion on readers’ comprehension. The results demonstrated that low level readers benefited more from high- cohesive texts whereas high level readers benefited more from low-cohesive texts. This is because high-cohesive texts employed many anaphoric referents, sentence connectives, background information, meaningful headings and paragraphs while low cohesive texts do not contain so much structuring information (Tubingen, 2011). Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 75 McNamara et al. (ibid) clarified that low- cohesive text required high level readers to engage in compensatory processing to infer unstated relations in the texts as germane possessing, while high cohesive text seduce high level readers to more passing processing instead of activating relevant prior knowledge of their own. In an effort to support the germane cognitive load explanation, O’Reilly and McNamara (2007) did a study about its effect on reading comprehension and found that learners with high prior knowledge and low reading skills did not benefit from high cohesive texts while skilled learners with high knowledge and reading skills would benefit from high cohesive texts. On explain their findings, O’Reilly and McNamara (ibid) considered that good reading skills assist high knowledge learners in involving in germane cognitive load processing. Kalyuga et al. (2007) explained that high knowledge learners, as skilled readers, know how to apply active processing strategies into well guided text instructions. McNamara et al. (ibid) stated that information added to an original biology instructional text for coherence enhancement was advantageous to low-knowledge readers only. However, an original minimally coherent format text was useful for high-knowledge readers more than an enhanced one. Unlike the study done by McNamara et al. (ibid), this experiment was conducted within the framework of CLT in which cognitive load approaches were used to measure effort and the efficiency. Accordingly, the current experiment used expanded and reduced versions instead of high-cohesive and low cohesive texts used by McNamara et al. (ibid) in their study. In the expanded and reduced versions, the sentences were added or removed while in the high- cohesive texts and low cohesive texts, the content of the versions were modified by changing cohesive devices. Though CLT has been introduced since 2007 (Huynh, 2007), no studies on cognitive load effects as expertise reversal effect have been carried out in the Vietnamese context. The paper is the first study in Vietnam to investigate the expertise reversal effect on EFL area related to reading comprehension. Based on a review of the study by McNamara et al (ibid), the ex ... .60 30 Reduced 42.0 20.5 30 Total 23.3 24.6 60 Total expanded 15.33 20.94 60 Reduced 23.66 24.56 60 Total 19.50 23.11 120 Figure 1. Performance scores in the learning phase Figure 2. Performance scores in the test phase 3030 3030N = GROUP expert groupnovice group L E A R N IN G 120 100 80 60 40 20 0 -20 VERSION expanded version reduced version 3030 3030N = GROUP expert groupnovice group T E S T 100 80 60 40 20 0 -20 VERSION expanded version reduced version 9610012 56214338 34 8520673 Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 79 Mental effort ratings (Table 2) demonstrated that the main effect of version was not significant, F (1,116) = .011, MSE = .747, p = .916. The main effect of expertise group was significant, F (1,116) = 22.5, MSE = .747, p < .001, partial Eta Squared = .163 (see Table 2). There was a significant interaction between the groups and versions, F (1,116) = 18.7, MSE = .747, p < .001, partial Eta Squared = .139. Simple effect tests revealed that the effort scores of the expanded version were higher than those of the reduced version for the expert group, F (1,116) = 9.83, MSE = .747, p = .002, partial Eta Squared = .078 while the effort scores of the reduced version were higher than those of the expanded version for the novice group, F (1,116) = 8.92, MSE =.747, p = .003, partial Eta Squared = .071 (Figure 3). According to Paas and Van Merrienboer (1993), an efficiency score can be generated by using the difference between the z score of performance and the z score of effort. The main effect of version was not significant, F (1,116) = 1.34, MSE = .921, p = .209. The main effect of expert groups was significant, F (1,116) = 21.4, MSE=.921 p < .001, partial Eta Squared = .156 (See Table 4). There was a significant interaction between the groups and versions, F (1,116) = 27.0. MSE = .921, p < .001, partial Eta Squared = .189. Simple effect tests indicated that the reduced version was relatively more efficient than expanded version for the expert group, F (1,116) = 20.2, MSE = .926, p < .001, partial Eta Squared =.148. In contrast, the expanded version was relatively more efficient than the reduced version for the novice group, F (1,116) =8.17, MSE = .926, p = .005, partial Eta Squared =.066 (Figure 4). Table 2 Effort and relative instructional efficiency in the experiment Group Version Effort Efficiency Mean SD Mean SD Expert Expanded 5.53 0.937 .0520 1.18703 Reduced 4.83 0.648 .8025 .83503 Total 5.18 0.837 .4273 1.08561 Novice Expanded 5.60 1.102 -.3472 .82794 Reduced 6.27 0.691 -.5074 .95412 Total 5.93 0.972 -.4273 .88933 Total Expanded 5.57 1.015 -. 1476 1.03442 Reduced 5.55 .982 .1476 1.17745 Total 5.56 .994 .0000 1.07729 80 Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 3030 3030N = GROUP novice groupexpert group S C O R E 7.5 7.0 6.5 6.0 5.5 5.0 4.5 4.0 3.5 VERSION expanded version reduced version Figure 3. Effort scores of the two groups Figure 4. Efficiency scores of the two groups 4. Discussion As expected, the results showed that in the learning phase there was a significant interaction between the two groups and the two versions. The results demonstrated that, for the expert group, the reduced version outperformed the expanded version. The experts had better English proficiency in Geography. Thus, the experts were able to answer the question quickly and accurately. To comprehend the reduced version, the experts found it easy to find key words and to answer the questions. However, for the expanded version, the experts found it more difficult to answer the questions because the information provided and added to the version were redundant and caused an extraneous cognitive load. The results of the experiment in the learning phase for experts were different from previous studies (Oblinger and Oblinger, 2005; Chujo and Utliyama, 2005) in which text length had no significant effect on 3030 3030N = GROUP NOVICE GROUPEXPERT GROUP S C O R E 3 2 1 0 -1 -2 -3 VERSION EXPANDED GROUP REDUCED GROUP 68 Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 81 reading comprehension. Contrary to expectation, in the learning phase, the results revealed that the expanded version did not significantly outperform the reduced version for the novices. The results in the learning phase for novices were consistent with some previous studies (Jalilehvand, 2012; Strother and Ulijn, 1987; Mehrpour and Riazi, 2004) which showed a non- significant effect of text length on reading comprehension. The results do not accord with McNamara et al’s (1996) data. Even though the expanded version had extra seven sentences explaining more about dinosaurs’ extinction, this addition seemed not enough to fill the gap between novice and experts’ background knowledge and the content of the text. One reason might be that English is the mother tongue of high school students in McNamara et al’s (1996) study while English is a foreign language for the Vietnamese students (ESP) in this experiment. Accordingly, the students of this experiment need to acquire adequate English proficiency as a second language to have appropriate background knowledge for understanding the content of the text. A second reason is that novices in the experiment were students from the Department of Mathematics who knew very little about geographical English, the domain of the text. For these two reasons, adding more information in the expanded version may not help the novices to achieve active processing in reading comprehension. In other words, the length of versions did not help novices to infer the content of the text in the learning phase. Nevertheless, the experiment results showed that both reduced and expanded versions did not make a difference for the novices in the learning phase. Unlike the “more effort” hypothesis suggested by McNamara et al. (ibid), the performance scores and effort scores of the experiment findings showed that the reduced version was beneficial to the experts as they need less effort to comprehend it. The reduced version in the experiment was a “challenging text” as McNamara et al. (ibid) suggested. The results of the mental effort scores also showed that McNamara’s hypothesis of putting in more effort was not supported. How much the experts understand the reduced version depend on how much they understand geography as the subject matter and whether knowledge they learnt from Department of Geography enough for them to comprehend the text. Obviously, the experts needed extra processing and differencing to understand the version. Next, the expanded version could help the novices to recall its content and answer the question because they already read this version in the learning phase and may have sufficient schemas to answer the questions in the test phase without looking at the text. In contrast, the novices who read the reduced version in the learning phase did not have sufficient schemas to recall the content and answer the questions precisely. For the experts, the background schemas helped them to recall more effectively. However, the experts who previously read the expanded version might have trouble recalling the content and answering the questions in the test phase due to extraneous and redundant cognitive load caused by the expanded version they learnt. The results of recalling in the test phase of this experiment were in line with that of MacNamara et al.’s (ibid) studies. Accordingly, the expanded version that helped the novices recall its content turned out to be counter-productive to the experts because it provided more appropriate schemas for novices to answer the questions while created a redundant and extraneous cognitive load for the experts. On the contrary, if novices did not get suitable schemas, it would generate an extraneous cognitive load for them to read the 82 Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 reduced version. Mental efforts scores showed significant interactions. It took more efforts for experts than for novices to understand the expanded version. These findings contradict with those of McNamara et al’s study. Also, instructional efficiency scores indicated that reduced version may be more efficient than expanded version for experts whereas the latter seemed more efficient than the former for novices. 5. Implications Regarding educational implications, the results of this paper suggest that the alternative versions of a text should be designed appropriately to readers’ knowledge. The use of suitable versions may be very effective in facilitating reading comprehension, especially in an EFL context. Instructors should not design a reading comprehension version that might impose an extraneous cognitive load for readers and do not enhance their reading comprehension skills. The current research has provided some further insights into the relevant constructs. Of the two reduced and expanded versions of a text, the reduced version had a significant positive influence on high level readers while the expanded version helped enhance reading comprehension for lower level readers. In fact, instructors often provide all students with one general version of a text without considering the difference in their levels of knowledge. The findings implied that teachers should give students adequate versions of a text to improve learners’ reading comprehension. Furthermore, the main results of the paper revealed that the expanded version of a text could benefit novices significantly and thus, should be designed in a way to improve their reading comprehension. In contrast, the reduced version of a text needs to be designed to enhance the reading process of experts. 6. Conclusion The present study found that reading instructions in ESP (English for Geography and History) used by readers of different levels could yield expertise reversal effect. The significant interaction between the two groups and two versions of the experiment indicated a negative correlation between the versions and the students’ expertise. The results showed that it was not the expanded version but the reduced version did enhance reading comprehension for experts because they were equipped with more sophisticated schemas for reading comprehension References Chi, M.T.H., Feltovich, P. & Glasser, R., (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121-152. Chujo, K. Utliyama, M. (2005). Understand the role of text length, sample size and vocabulary size in determining text coverage. Reading in a foreign language, 17(1). Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1983). Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 561-584. Eskey, E., & Grabe, W. (1988). Interactive models for second language reading: perspectives on instruction. In P. Carrell., J. Devine, & E..Eskey (Eds.) Interactive Approaches to Second Language (pp. 223-237). Reading, Cambridge University Press. Feather, R.M., Snyder, S.L. (1993). Earth Science. McMillan. Goldman, S.R., Varma, S., & Cote, N. (1996). Extending capacity constrained construction interaction: Toward “smarter” and flexible models of text comprehension: In B.K. Britton & A.C. Greasser (Eds.), Models of understanding text, pp.73-113., Mahvaly, NJ: Erlbaum. Huynh Cong Minh Hung. Journal of Science Ho Chi Minh City Open University, 7(4), 74-83 83 Huynh, C.M.H. (2007). Enhancing reading comprehension with an integrated format. 32 nd Annual conference of applied linguistics Association of Australia (ALAA 2007). The University of Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia. Jalilehvand, M. (2012). The effects of text length and picture on reading comprehension on Iranian EFL students. Asian Social Science. 3(3), 329-337. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2007). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23-31. Kalyuga, S., Renkl, A. (2010). Expertise reversal effect and its instructional implications: introduction to the special issue. Instructional Science, 38, 209-215. McNamara, D.S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N.B., Kintsch, W., (1996). Are good texts always better? Interaction of text coherence, background knowledge, and level of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 1-43. Mehrpour, S. & Riazi, A. (2004). The impact of text length on EFL students’ reading comprehension. Asia EFL journal. Oblinger, D.G., & Oblinger, J.L. (2005). Educating the net generation. Educause. Oksa, A., Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P. (2010). Expertise reversal effect in using explanatory notes for readers of Shakespearean text. Instructional Science, 38, 217-236. Paas, F.W.C., & van Merrienboer (2003), The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine mental effort and performance measures. Human factors, 35, 737-743. Perfetti, C.A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press. Strother, J.B. & Ulijn, J.M. (1987). Does syntactic rewriting affect English for science and technology text comprehension?. In J. Devine, P.L. Carrell and D.E. Eskey (Eds.). Research in reading in English as a second language. Washington, DC: TESOL, 91-101.
File đính kèm:
- expertise_reversal_effect_on_reading_comprehension_a_case_of.pdf